International Journal of Yoga
Users online: 314 
Ahead of print | Login 
 
Home Bookmark this page Print this page Email this page Small font sizeDefault font size Increase font size 
About us Editors Current Issue Past Issues Instructions submission Subscribe Advertise
 


 
   Table of Contents     
SHORT COMMUNICATION  
Year : 2019  |  Volume : 12  |  Issue : 3  |  Page : 247-251
Performance in a Corsi block-tapping task following high-frequency yoga breathing or breath awareness


Department of Yoga Research, Patanjali Research Foundation (Trust), Haridwar, Uttarakhand, India

Click here for correspondence address and email

Date of Web Publication3-Sep-2019
 

   Abstract 


Background: Uninostril yoga breathing practices have improved spatial memory scores. There has been no assessment on the effect of high-frequency yoga breathing (HFYB) on working memory and spatial memory scores using the Corsi block-tapping task (CBTT). Objectives: The present study was planned to assess the immediate effects of HFYB and breath awareness (BAW) compared to a control session on performance in a CBTT. Methods: Fifteen participants of both sexes with ages between 18 and 24 years (group mean age ± standard deviation, 20.0 ± 1.6 years; 10 females) were recruited for the trial from a university in North India. Each participant was assessed in three sessions conducted on 3 separate days at the same time of the day. The three sessions were (i) HFYB, (ii) BAW, and (iii) quiet sitting (QS). The duration of the intervention was 18 min. The participants were assessed before and after all the three sessions. Repeated-measures-analyses of variance followed by post hoc tests with Bonferroni adjustment were performed to compare data before and after all the three sessions. Results: BAW resulted in an improvement in backward total scores (P < 0.05) and the backward Corsi span (P < 0.05; one tailed). Conclusions: The results suggest that BAW improves primary working memory, spatial memory, and spatial attention. HFYB did not cause any change.

Keywords: Breath awareness, Corsi block-tapping task, high-frequency yoga breathing

How to cite this article:
Gupta RK, Agnihotri S, Telles S, Balkrishna A. Performance in a Corsi block-tapping task following high-frequency yoga breathing or breath awareness. Int J Yoga 2019;12:247-51

How to cite this URL:
Gupta RK, Agnihotri S, Telles S, Balkrishna A. Performance in a Corsi block-tapping task following high-frequency yoga breathing or breath awareness. Int J Yoga [serial online] 2019 [cited 2019 Sep 19];12:247-51. Available from: http://www.ijoy.org.in/text.asp?2019/12/3/247/265739



   Introduction Top


Voluntary breath regulation (pranayama in Sanskrit) consists of yoga practices which modify the depth, rate, and the other dimensions of respiration.[1] The breath rate can be voluntarily increased as high as 2.0 Hz in a practice called kapalabhati (kapala = forehead; bhati = shining in Sanskrit), described as high-frequency yoga breathing (HFYB). HFYB is a yoga breathing technique which is practiced by increasing the breath rate and forceful exhalation.

All pranayama practices include breath awareness (BAW) as an important component to direct the attention inward and ultimately reduce awareness of all other stimuli and sensations.[2] Both HFYB and BAW have been shown to influence attention. The performance in a cancellation task on 110 participants (Cohen's d = 0.99) whose age ranging from 18 years to over 60 years was improved after 1 min of practice of HFYB at 2.0 Hz. The results suggest an improvement in ability to shift attention and in selective and sustained attention.[3] Similarly, in another study, on assessments with the P300 event-related potential task, there was a decrease in P300 peak latency in thirty practitioners (Cohen's d = 0.61) after 1 min of the practice of HFYB at 2.0 Hz.[4] These results suggested that HFYB reduced the time taken to complete the P300 task which measures selective attention. In the similar study, the practice of BAW showed an increase in the P300 peak amplitude which indicates an increase in neurons recruited to perform the task.[4] The results suggested beneficial effects of these practices (i.e., HFYB and BAW) on conscious arousal and attention.

Mindfulness does bear some resemblance with BAW. In a study, 18 long-term practitioners showed an improvement in attention, working memory, and cognitive flexibility,[5] when they were compared to age-matched control group.

One dimension of attention is visuospatial attention which involves directing the attention to the location of an object in space. After directing attention to a specific spatial location, further cognitive processing may be required, which may involve working memory.[6]

The Corsi block-tapping task (CBTT) is a nonverbal test[7] to measure executive functions such as visuospatial short-term memory, working memory,[8] and spatial attention.[9]

Yoga practice including pranayamas improved spatial memory scores by 43% when thirty schoolchildren (Cohen's d = 0.89) with ages between 11 and 16 years were assessed using verbal and spatial memory tests.[10] Similar effects were found when four specific types of pranayamas improved spatial memory scores by 84% in schoolchildren.[11]

However, to the knowledge of the authors, no study has attempted to study the effects of an individual pranayama on visuospatial memory using CBTT, which is a more objective way of assessing primary working memory. Hence, the present study was designed to assess the immediate effects of HFYB and BAW on performance in CBTT in comparison with a control session.


   Methods Top


Participants

Fifteen volunteers of both sexes with ages between 18 and 24 years (group mean age ± standard deviation (SD), 20.0 ± 1.6 years; 10 females) were recruited as participants. Because menstrual cycle affects different aspects of attention,[12] female participants were assessed after 7 days of their period of menstrual cycle. The sample size was based on changes in backward total scores after BAW gave an effect size = 0.52 (medium) and power = 0.95 using G Power software version 3.1, Germany.[13] The inclusion criteria were (i) minimum 3 months of experience in the yoga breathing practices, (ii) ability to practice HFYB at the rate of 1.0 Hz (range, 0.83–1.17 Hz); their breath rate was checked using Quark cardio pulmonary exercise test, and (iii) normal health based on a routine clinical examination. The exclusion criteria were (i) any history of epilepsy, (ii) recent chest or abdominal surgery (because the intervention was high-frequency breathing), (iii) taking medication or using other wellness strategies, and (iv) use of stimulants or intoxicating substances. The study conditions were explained to the participants. Their signed informed consent was written in English and explained in both English and Hindi for those who were not able to understand English adequately. The study was approved by the institutional ethics committee (approval number YRD/017/032).

Study design

Each participant was assessed in all the three sessions on 3 separate days at the same time of the day. These sessions were (i) HFYB, (ii) BAW, and (iii) quiet sitting (QS) as a control group. The sequence of the practices was randomized for 15 participants using a randomizer (www.randomizer.org). The total duration of each session was 18 min, with 1 min of rest after each 5 min of practice. During the sessions, participants were asked to sit erect and keep their eyes closed.

Assessment

Corsi block-tapping task

The CBTT is an individual test which measures visuospatial short-term and working memory.[8] Nine blue squares appear on the screen. For each trial, the squares “light up” as yellow one by one in a varying sequence. After the presentation, the participants had to click each of the boxes in the similar order in which they had “lit up” the first part of the task, i.e. forward tapping. In the second part of the task, they maintained the reverse order, i.e., backward tapping. The task begins with a two-box sequence to a maximum of nine. The test gets terminated when the participant is not able to remember the sequence for two consecutive trials at any one level. Hence, the test assesses the following four variables: (i) forward Corsi span, (ii) forward total score, (iii) backward Corsi span, and (iv) the backward total score. The sample of the square presentation along with forward and backward tapping is shown in [Figure 1].
Figure 1: Sequence of the square presentation along with forward and backward tapping

Click here to view


Intervention

Each participant performed three yoga techniques namely (1) HFYB, (2) BAW, and (3) QS. The total time for each intervention session was 18 min, with 1 min of rest after each 5 min of all the sessions.

High-frequency yoga breathing

The participants were asked to sit in a comfortable posture (sukhasana), keeping their spine erect, neck aligned, and eyes closed. They practiced HFYB at approximately 1.0 Hz with forceful exhalation, for 3 periods of 5 min each.

Breath awareness

The participants were asked to be aware of the flow of the air as it enters and passes through the nasal passages without modifying their breathing. The participants were asked to maintain similar posture in all the three sessions, seated with spine erect and eyes closed.

Quiet sitting

In the QS as a control session, the participants allowed their thoughts to wander at random without modifying them or being aware of their breathing. They were asked to sit with their spine erect and eyes closed. No other activity was given to them.

Data analysis

Repeated-measures-analyses of variance was performed for (i) forward Corsi span, (ii) forward total score, (iii) backward Corsi span, and (iv) backward total score in which the three sessions (HFYB, BAW, and QS) and two states (before and after sessions) were compared. Post hoc analysis was performed to compare data before and after all the three sessions which were Bonferroni adjusted.


   Results Top


BAW improved total scores in backward tapping (P < 0.05) and backward Corsi span (P < 0.05; one tailed). HFYB did not show any changes in Corsi span and total scores of backward as well as forward tapping. Changes in mean, SD, and Cohen's d for both forward and backward Corsi span with total scores are summarized in [Table 1]. The df, mean square, F, and partial eta squared values are summarized in [Table 2].
Table 1: Mean, SD, and Cohen's d for forward and backward Corsi span with total scores

Click here to view
Table 2: Details of four repeated-measures-analyses of variance

Click here to view



   Discussion Top


Fifteen minutes of BAW resulted in an improvement in the backward total scores and backward Corsi span using the CBTT. This suggested an improvement in executive functions in terms of increased visuospatial short-term memory, primary working memory,[8] and specific spatial processes.[14]

The forward digit span test measures material-specific slave systems while the backward test measures primarily tax central executive resources.[15]

Previously, attention was improved during and after HFYB and BAW using the P300 event-related potential task in thirty male adult volunteers[4] who had experience in yoga breathing practices for at least 3 months. Similar improvement after BAW occurred when performance in a letter cancellation task was assessed in twenty male volunteers whose ages ranged from 20 to 45 years.[16] The findings suggested that BAW can improve performance in attention tasks.[16]

In the studies cited above, attention was measured using the P300 event-related potential task and a letter cancellation task, whereas in the present study, visuospatial attention was measured using the CBTT. As described below, yoga breathing practices or pranayamas were found to improve performance in recall in the digit span test.

Eighty-four participants with an age range between 18 and 25 years showed an improvement in both forward and backward digit span tests suggestive of an improvement in executive functions after practicing a combination of yoga breathing practices.[17] In another study, 67 healthy medical students (37 females) with an age range of 18–25 years showed an improvement in both forward and backward digit span tests after 4 weeks of practicing a combination of bhramari, anulom-vilom, kapalabhati, and bhastrika for 30 min per day.[18] Digit span test differs from the CBTT in the brain regions involved[19] and cognitive activities.[8]

BAW does bear some resemblance to mindfulness. Tomasino and Fabbro showed that 8 weeks of mindfulness meditation increased activity in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) and in the left caudate/anterior insula, while activation in the rostral PFC and right parietal area 3b (subdivision of the brodmann area) reduced.[20]

The dorsolateral PFC was found to be associated with processing of information such as integrating different dimensions of cognition and behavior,[21] ability to maintain and shift set, planning and working memory.[22],[23]

Another possible mechanism for the improvement in attention after BAW could be increased sympathetic and decreased parasympathetic activities.[24] This is relevant because attention and vigilance are associated with higher sympathetic modulation in human volunteers.[25]

The findings were limited by (i) the small sample size (n = 15) though the power was 0.95 which is high compared to an earlier study, (ii) recruitment of both sexes, and (iii) the levels of significance in all cases were low (P < 0.05; one tailed) and Cohen's d values were small. Unlike earlier studies, the participants in the present study were not assessed using the standard digit span task, which is a limitation when attempting to view these findings based on the CBTT in light of earlier studies which used the digit span task. A possible future direction of research could be simultaneous assessment of the CBTT performance with the digit span test.


   Conclusions Top


BAW significantly improved backward total scores and backward Corsi span, suggesting an improvement in primary working memory, spatial memory, and spatial attention.

Financial support and sponsorship

This study was financially supported by Patanjali Research Foundation (Trust).

Conflicts of interest

There are no conflicts of interest.



 
   References Top

1.
Telles S, Singh N. A review of the use of yoga in breathing disorders. In: Chaitow L, Gilbert C, Morrison D, editors. Recognizing and Treating Breathing Disorders – A Multidisciplinary Approach. United Kingdom: Elsevier; 2013. p. 275-82.  Back to cited text no. 1
    
2.
Available from: https://www.ekhartyoga.com/articles/the-importance-of-breath-in-yoga. [Last accessed on 2018 Jan 05].  Back to cited text no. 2
    
3.
Telles S, Raghuraj P, Arankalle D, Naveen KV. Immediate effect of high-frequency yoga breathing on attention. Indian J Med Sci 2008;62:20-2.  Back to cited text no. 3
[PUBMED]  [Full text]  
4.
Joshi M, Telles S. A nonrandomized non-naive comparative study of the effects of kapalabhati and breath awareness on event-related potentials in trained yoga practitioners. J Altern Complement Med 2009;15:281-5.  Back to cited text no. 4
    
5.
Vecera SP, Rizzo M. Spatial attention: Normal processes and their breakdown. Neurol Clin 2003;21:575-607.  Back to cited text no. 5
    
6.
Fabio RA, Towey GE. Long-term meditation: The relationship between cognitive processes, thinking styles and mindfulness. Cogn Process 2018;19:73-85.  Back to cited text no. 6
    
7.
Berch DB, Krikorian R, Huha EM. The Corsi Block-tapping task: Methodological and theoretical considerations. Brain Cogn 1998;38:317-38.  Back to cited text no. 7
    
8.
Kessels RP, van den Berg E, Ruis C, Brands AM. The backward span of the Corsi Block-tapping task and its association with the WAIS-III digit span. Assessment 2008;15:426-34.  Back to cited text no. 8
    
9.
Smyth MM, Scholey KA. Interference in immediate spatial memory. Mem Cognit 1994;22:1-3.  Back to cited text no. 9
    
10.
Manjunath NK, Telles S. Spatial and verbal memory test scores following yoga and fine arts camps for school children. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol 2004;48:353-6.  Back to cited text no. 10
    
11.
Naveen KV, Nagarathna R, Nagendra HR, Telles S. Yoga breathing through a particular nostril increases spatial memory scores without lateralized effects. Psychol Rep 1997;81:555-61.  Back to cited text no. 11
    
12.
Pletzer B, Harris TA, Ortner T. Sex and menstrual cycle influences on three aspects of attention. Physiol Behav 2017;179:384-90.  Back to cited text no. 12
    
13.
Erdfelder E, Faul F, Buchner A. GPOWER: A general power analysis program. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 1996;28:1-11.  Back to cited text no. 13
    
14.
Mammarella IC, Cornoldi C. Sequence and space: The critical role of a backward spatial span in the working memory deficit of visuospatial learning disabled children. Cogn Neuropsychol 2005;22:1055-68.  Back to cited text no. 14
    
15.
Monaco M, Costa A, Caltagirone C, Carlesimo GA. Forward and backward span for verbal and visuo-spatial data: Standardization and normative data from an Italian adult population. Neurol Sci 2013;34:749-54.  Back to cited text no. 15
    
16.
Telles S, Raghuraj P, Maharana S, Nagendra HR. Immediate effect of three yoga breathing techniques on performance on a letter-cancellation task. Percept Mot Skills 2007;104:1289-96.  Back to cited text no. 16
    
17.
Sharma VK, M R, S V, Subramanian SK, Bhavanani AB, Madanmohan, et al. Effect of fast and slow pranayama practice on cognitive functions in healthy volunteers. J Clin Diagn Res 2014;8:10-3.  Back to cited text no. 17
    
18.
Jain R, Tonpay PS. Effect of pranayama on cognitive functions of medical students. Indian J Basic Appl Med Res 2016;6:471-6.  Back to cited text no. 18
    
19.
Milner B. Interhemispheric differences in the localization of psychological processes in man. Br Med Bull 1971;27:272-7.  Back to cited text no. 19
    
20.
Tomasino B, Fabbro F. Increases in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and decreases the rostral prefrontal cortex activation after-8 weeks of focused attention based mindfulness meditation. Brain Cogn 2016;102:46-54.  Back to cited text no. 20
    
21.
Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Bigler ED, Tranel D. Neuropsychological Assessment. 4th ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004.  Back to cited text no. 21
    
22.
Alvarez JA, Emory E. Executive function and the frontal lobes: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychol Rev 2006;16:17-42.  Back to cited text no. 22
    
23.
Clark L, Bechara A, Damasio H, Aitken MR, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. Differential effects of insular and ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions on risky decision-making. Brain 2008;131:1311-22.  Back to cited text no. 23
    
24.
Telles S, Singh N, Balkrishna A. Heart rate variability changes during high frequency yoga breathing and breath awareness. Biopsychosoc Med 2011;5:4.  Back to cited text no. 24
    
25.
Peyrin L, Pequignot JM, Lacour JR, Fourcade J. Relationships between catecholamine or 3-methoxy 4-hydroxy phenylglycol changes and the mental performance under submaximal exercise in man. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1987;93:188-92.  Back to cited text no. 25
    

Top
Correspondence Address:
Mr Ram Kumar Gupta
Patanjali Research Foundation, Patanjali Yogpeeth, Haridwar - 249 405, Uttarakhand
India
Login to access the Email id

Source of Support: None, Conflict of Interest: None


DOI: 10.4103/ijoy.IJOY_55_18

Rights and Permissions


    Figures

  [Figure 1]
 
 
    Tables

  [Table 1], [Table 2]



 

Top
 
 
  Search
 
    Similar in PUBMED
    Search Pubmed for
    Search in Google Scholar for
  Related articles
    Email Alert *
    Add to My List *
* Registration required (free)  


    Abstract
   Introduction
   Methods
   Results
   Discussion
   Conclusions
    References
    Article Figures
    Article Tables

 Article Access Statistics
    Viewed51    
    Printed0    
    Emailed0    
    PDF Downloaded0    
    Comments [Add]    

Recommend this journal