International Journal of Yoga
Users online: 171 
Ahead of print | Login 
 
Home Bookmark this page Print this page Email this page Small font sizeDefault font size Increase font size 
About us Editors Current Issue Past Issues Instructions submission Subscribe Advertise
 


 
   Table of Contents     
ORIGINAL ARTICLE  
Year : 2012  |  Volume : 5  |  Issue : 1  |  Page : 37-41
Finger dexterity and visual discrimination following two yoga breathing practices


Department of Yoga Research, Patanjali Research Foundation, Patanjali Yogpeeth, Haridwar, India

Click here for correspondence address and email

Date of Web Publication11-Jan-2012
 

   Abstract 

Background: Practicing yoga has been shown to improve motor functions and attention. Though attention is required for fine motor and discrimination tasks, the effect of yoga breathing techniques on fine motor skills and visual discrimination has not been assessed.
Aim: To study the effect of yoga breathing techniques on finger dexterity and visual discrimination.
Materials and Methods: The present study consisted of one hundred and forty subjects who had enrolled for stress management. They were randomly divided into two groups, one group practiced high frequency yoga breathing while the other group practiced breath awareness. High frequency yoga breathing (kapalabhati, breath rate 1.0 Hz) and breath awareness are two yoga practices which improve attention. The immediate effect of high frequency yoga breathing and breath awareness (i) were assessed on the performance on the O'Connor finger dexterity task and (ii) (in) a shape and size discrimination task.
Results: There was a significant improvement in the finger dexterity task by 19% after kapalabhati and 9% after breath awareness (P<0.001 in both cases, repeated measures ANOVA and post-hoc analyses). There was a significant reduction (P<0.001) in error (41% after kapalabhati and 21% after breath awareness) as well as time taken to complete the shape and size discrimination test (15% after kapalabhati and 15% after breath awareness; P<0.001) was also observed.
Conclusion: Both kapalabahati and breath awareness can improve fine motor skills and visual discrimination, with a greater magnitude of change after kapalabhati.

Keywords: Finger dexterity; shape and size discrimination; yoga breathing

How to cite this article:
Telles S, Singh N, Balkrishna A. Finger dexterity and visual discrimination following two yoga breathing practices. Int J Yoga 2012;5:37-41

How to cite this URL:
Telles S, Singh N, Balkrishna A. Finger dexterity and visual discrimination following two yoga breathing practices. Int J Yoga [serial online] 2012 [cited 2019 Oct 15];5:37-41. Available from: http://www.ijoy.org.in/text.asp?2012/5/1/37/91710

   Introduction Top


Perception is often discussed with reference to cues as a separate source of information for the perceiver. [1] These cues are in turn correlated with the manner in which the sensory apparatus has physically and computationally evolved. Sensory perception and discrimination of shapes and sizes depends on haptic or tactile cues. [2] Tactile information relevant to the size discrimination is combined with proprioceptive inputs. [3] Hence shape and size detection requires specific stereotypical movements. [4] These movements often take the form of scanning movements of the fingers. [5] While not directly related to the kind of movements mentioned above, finger movements involved in performing a dexterity task are also required for haptic sensitivity; the two (i.e. size discrimination and dexterity) being influenced by similar factors. [6]

Yoga is an ancient science, originating in India which includes specific postures, voluntary breath regulation, meditation, and certain philosophical principles. [7] Practicing yoga has been shown to influence several motor functions. These include static motor performance, [8],[9] tweezer dexterity, [10],[11] maze learning, [12] as well as visuomotor speed. [13]

Early studies on experienced practitioners of Burmese Buddhist meditation showed that experienced meditation practitioners were more sensitive to various aspects of visual stimuli, such as size, shape, color and texture. [14] Apart from meditation alone, a combination of yoga practices was shown to improve the sensitivity to a flickering light stimulus, [15] to reduce visual geometric illusions, [16] as well as to improve visual contrast sensitivity. [17]

More recently Tai Chi, a Chinese slow-motion meditation was shown to enhance tactile acuity in long-term practitioners. [18] A blinded assessor compared the ability to discriminate between two different orientations (i.e., parallel and horizontal) of different cross-grating widths at the fingertips in Tai Chi practitioners and controls. This tactile spatial acuity was found to be better in more experienced Tai Chi practitioners, compared to those with less experience in meditation. The authors also speculated that related somatosensory attentional practices such as yoga mindfulness meditation, and qigong could have similar effects. However, there has been no study on the effect of yoga on tactile acuity based on the performance in a size and shape discrimination task. Hence the present study was planned to assess the short term (or immediate) effect of two yoga breathing practices on (i) finger dexterity and (ii) size and shape discrimination.

The yoga techniques studied were two breathing practices. One of them was a high frequency yoga breathing practice(s), called kapalabhati (where kapala=forehead, bhati=shining, in Sanskrit). [19] Immediately after this practice, university students, middle-aged adults and persons 60 years of age and older, performed better in a cancellation task. [20] The letter cancellation task assesses selective and sustained attention, shifting attention, as well as visual scanning. [21] There was no change when the same participants practiced breath awareness (as control) to assess re-test effects on another day. Breath awareness was chosen in the study cited above, as well as in the present study as a control as breath awareness is an important part of all yoga voluntary regulated breathing and in traditional yoga.

Hence, in summary the present study assessed the immediate effects of (i) high frequency yoga breathing (kapalabhati) and (ii) breath awareness, on (i) size and shape discrimination and (ii) finger dexterity.


   Materials and Methods Top


Participants

The participants were attending an outpatient department to receive treatment in yoga and ayurveda at Patanjali yogpeeth, Haridwar, India. All participants were also given an opportunity to learn yoga in the outpatient facility area. A general announcement was made in the out-patient department and interested subjects who were seeking stress management enrolled and expressed their interest to volunteer. All of them gave their signed consent to participate in the study, which was approved by the Ethics Committee of Patanjali Research Foundation.

There were ninety-six subjects ranging in age between 20 and 55 years. The participants had enrolled for the yoga program from stress management. They were randomly allocated to two groups namely, high frequency yoga breathing HFYB (kapalabhati) group and the breath awareness group, using a random number table. Hand dominance of all the participants was checked using the Edinburgh handedness inventory. [22] During handedness testing, it was found that one person from the high frequency yoga breathing group was left hand dominant, and this person was exempted from the study. For ease of analysis and to make the groups equal in number, one more person (selected at random) who was allocated to the breath awareness group was excluded from the study. Hence the number of participants was ninety-four. Out of ninety-four patients, forty-seven patients, (28 females and 19 males) were taken as the high frequency yoga breathing group (group average age±S.D., 39.3 ± 10.5 years). The remaining forty-seven participants, (26 females and 21 males) were assigned to the breath awareness group (group average age±S.D., 39.8 ± 10.7). None of the participants had any active form of disease nor did they have any physical disability which would have interfered with their performance on the task. All of them had seven days of experience for both high frequency yoga breathing and breath awareness exercises, which they practiced every day (during the seven days).

Design

The ninety-four participants were randomized as two groups, with forty seven participants in each group. The sample size (i.e., 47 in each group) was not calculated prior to the test. However, the G*Power (a general power analysis program) 3.1 [23] software was used to calculate the power of the test as a post-hoc analysis. The power was found to be adequate i.e., 0.9997 (for the finger dexterity task) and 0.9998 (for the shape and size discrimination task). One group practiced high frequency yoga breathing while the other group practiced breath awareness. The two groups were comparable for age and gender. The detailed characteristics of the two groups are given in [Table 1].
Table 1: Details of the participants of both groups

Click here to view


Assessments

All participants were assessed using two tasks. These are described below:

O'Connor finger dexterity task

Finger dexterity was measured using the O'Connor finger dexterity apparatus, which consists of a shallow tray beside a metal plate which has 100 holes arranged in ten rows of ten holes each. The participants had to place three cylindrical metal pins 2.5 cm long and 0.2 cm in diameter in each hole. [24] The number of pins placed in 4 min was counted.

Shape and size discrimination test

The shape and size discrimination task (Takei Scientific Instruments Co., Ltd., T.K.K.117 Coin Sorting Model, Japan) assesses a participant's ability to discriminate between coins of different sizes depending on their visual and tactile sensations as well as their speed of sorting. The participant has to insert fifty small coins, which are different in size and thickness into one of the five slits, where each slit is designed to allow a specific coin to pass through. The time taken to complete the task was recorded with a stopwatch.

Intervention


The high frequency yoga breathing (HFYB) group was asked to practice 10 min of high frequency yoga breathing (kapalabhati) where the person had to breathe with a frequency of approximately 1.0 Hz, during which the exhalation is an intentionally active process. The other group practiced breath awareness for the same period. During breath awareness, the participants maintained awareness of spontaneous breathing without any intentional manipulation of the nostrils, throat or chest. The participants' attention was directed to the natural movement of air into and out of their nostrils. They also attempted to be aware of the air flow and its' temperature as it moved through their nasal passages.

Data analysis

(i) Finger dexterity and (ii) shape and size discrimination scores obtained before and after high frequency yoga breathing (kapalabhati) practice and before and after breath awareness were compared using repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVA), with one between-subjects factor (i.e. Groups, with two levels, high frequency yoga breathing (kapalabhati) group and breath awareness group), and one within-subjects factor (i.e., states, with two levels, before and after).

Post-hoc analyses with multiple comparisons and Bonferroni adjustment (When multiple comparisons are carried out statistically, there is an increase in the risk of false positives, the Bonferroni corrections reduced this risk) was carried out to compare values recorded before and after high frequency yoga breathing (kapalabhati), as well as before and after breath awareness.


   Results Top


Repeated measures analysis of variance

The finger dexterity scores showed a significant difference between states (i.e. before and after, with F=138.37, df=1, 46.0, P<0.001) and between Groups (i.e. high frequency yoga breathing and breath awareness with F=5.13, df=1, 46.0, P<0.001).

The error scores in the shape and size discrimination test also showed a significant difference in the interaction between Groups and states (F=51.25, df=1, 46.0, P<0.001). This suggests that the two are interrelated. There was no significant difference between groups (i.e. high frequency yoga breathing and breath awareness). The time taken to complete the test also showed a significant difference between states (i.e. before and after with F=88.84, df=1, 46.0, P<0.001) and no significant difference was found between groups. In all cases, the Hyunh-Feldt epsilon was equal to 1 (Symmetry is an assumption that the numbers in the analysis of variance (ANOVA) matrix are homogenous, or almost nearly so. If the data do not meet the criteria for sphericity correction factors have to be used. An example of such a correction factor is the Huynh-Feldt Epsilon).

Post-hoc comparisons

Multiple post-hoc comparisons were carried out with Bonferroni adjustment. The scores of finger dexterity were significantly higher following high frequency yoga breathing (kapalabhati) as well as breath awareness compared to before (P<0.001, two tailed).

The percentage change after high frequency yoga breathing (kapalabhati) practice was 19.0% whereas in the case of breath awareness the increase was 9.0% after the practice.

There was a significant reduction in the error rate as well as time taken to complete the shape and size discrimination test after both practices i.e. high frequency yoga breathing (kapalabhati) and breath awareness (P<0.001, two tailed). Errors were reduced by 41% after high frequency yoga breathing (kapalabhati) practice whereas after practicing breath awareness the errors were reduced by 22%. The difference between the two groups, namely, kapalabhati (41% change) and breath awareness (22% change) can be explained based on studies which have shown that kapalabhati compared to breath awareness increases attention more [25] and increases visual perceptual activity more as well. [26] Apart from this while breath awareness appears to increase sympathetic tone based on the LF/HF ratio of the heart rate variability spectrum, kapalabhati does not have this effect. [27] This may be due to the fact that breath awareness involves a certain degree of focusing which is known to increase sympathetic tone. The time taken was reduced by 15% after high frequency yoga breathing (kapalabhati) as well as after practicing breath awareness (it reduced by 15%). The mean values±S.D. of finger dexterity test and shape and size discrimination tasks before and after kapalabhati and breath awareness are given in [Table 2].
Table 2: Comparison of kapalabhati and breath awareness on finger dexterity and shape and size discrimination tasks

Click here to view



   Discussion Top


In the present study, there was a significant improvement in finger dexterity following kapalabhati and breath awareness. The O'Connor finger dexterity task is a fine motor skill task which measures fingertip dexterity and eye-hand co-ordination. The improvement observed in the present study might have resulted due to the beneficial effect of kapalabhati on conscious arousal, attention [28] and anxiety based on changes in electroencephalography. [29] The present group of subjects were possibly anxious as they had joined for stress relief. However, their level of anxiety was not measured. Kapalabhati has been shown to improve attention in a study which assessed the effect of the practice on the P300 event related potential. [28] Attention has been shown to be a predictor of fine motor skills particularly in children suffering from attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. [30] Anxiety is also considered to influence motor skills. [31] Hence the improvements in attention and anxiety relief following kapalabhati practice as seen above might explain the increase in finger dexterity in the present study. This explanation is also applicable to the change seen after breath awareness as mindfulness meditation (which breath awareness resembles) is reported to improve attention [32] and reduce anxiety. [33]

The improvement in shape and size discrimination following kapalabhati found in the present study is in line with an earlier study which showed improvement in selective and sustained attention, as well as visual scanning following yoga breathing other than kapalabhati. [20] In a study on the effect of kapalabhati on spontaneous electrical activity in the brain, there was an increase in alpha, theta and beta 1 activity during the practice in the occipital region. [29] This change might have influenced the functioning of the extrastriate cortex in the occipital region which helps in visuospatial processing. [34] As the shape and size discrimination task involves visuospatial processing, the change in extrastriate cortical functioning may underlie the improvement following kapalabhati. Closely related to this, is the finding that 15 min of kapalabhati practice reduced error in visual optical illusion, when the Müller-Lyer lines were used for the assessments. [26] However, these explanations are merely a speculation at this stage as the exact mechanism of kapalabhati influencing tactile discrimination was not studied. The effect of kapalabhati on attention-arousal as mentioned earlier might also contribute to the improvement. Like kapalabhati, breath awareness has also been shown to improve the performance in an attention task for sustained, selective attention and visual scanning. [21] Interestingly, both kapalabhati and breath awareness did not appear to increase sympathetic tone, based on the LF/HF ratio in a frequency domain analysis of the heart rate variability spectrum in an earlier study [27] a change normally associated with increased attention-arousal. This was confirmed by studies on breath awareness [35] and kapalabhati. [27]


   Conclusion Top


In summary, both kapalabhati and breath awareness improved fine motor skills measured by finger dexterity and visual discrimination assessed by the shape and size discrimination task. While this may be related to earlier demonstrated effects on attention-arousal, the exact neural mechanisms are not yet known.


   Acknowledgement Top


The authors gratefully acknowledge Ms. Nilima Kumar Manjhi for her help in data collection.

 
   References Top

1.Ernst MO, Bülthoff HH. Merging the senses into a robust percept. Trends Cogn Sci 2004;8:162-9.  Back to cited text no. 1
    
2.Sherick CE. Touch as a communicative sense. J Acoust Soc Am 1985;77:218-9.  Back to cited text no. 2
    
3.Voisin J, Lammare Y, Chapman CE. Haptic discrimination of object shape in humans: Contribution of cutaneous and proprioceptive inputs. Exp Brain Res 2002;145:251-60.  Back to cited text no. 3
    
4.Lederman SJ, Klatzky RL. Extracting object properties through haptic exploration. Acta Psychologica 1993;84:29-40.  Back to cited text no. 4
[PUBMED]    
5.Grunwald M (Editor.). Human haptic pereption - basics and applications. Berlin, Germany: Birkhäuser; 2008.  Back to cited text no. 5
    
6.Woods JA, Leslie, LF, Drake DB, Edlich RF. Effect of puncture resistant surgical gloves, finger guards, and glove liners on cutaneous sensibility and surgical psychomotor skills. J Biomed Mater Res 1996;33:47-51.  Back to cited text no. 6
    
7.Taimini IK. The Science of Yoga. Madras, India: Theosophical Publishing House; 1986.  Back to cited text no. 7
    
8.Telles S, Hanumanthaih B, Nagarathna R, Nagendra HR. Improvement in static motor performance following yogic training of school children. Percept Mot Skills 1993;76:1264-6.  Back to cited text no. 8
    
9.Telles S, Hanumanthaih B, Nagarathna R, Nagendra HR. Plasticity of motor control systems demonstrated by yoga training. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol 1994;38:143-4.  Back to cited text no. 9
    
10.Manjunath NK, Telles S. Factors influencing changes in tweezer dexterity scores following yoga training. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol 1999;43:225-9.  Back to cited text no. 10
    
11.Lynton H, Kligler B, Shiflett S. Yoga in stroke rehabilitation: A systematic review and results of a pilot study. Top Stroke Rehabil 2007;14:1-8.  Back to cited text no. 11
    
12.Telles S, Ramaprabhu V, Reddy SK. Effect of yoga training on maze learning. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol 2000;44:197-201.  Back to cited text no. 12
    
13.Sharma VK, Das S, Mondal S, Goswami U, Gandhi A. Effect of Sahaj yoga on neuro-cognitive functions in patients suffering from major depression. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol 2006;50:375-83.  Back to cited text no. 13
    
14.Brown DP. A model for the levels of concentrative meditation. Int J Clin Exp Hypn 1977;25:236-73.  Back to cited text no. 14
    
15.Vani PR, Nagarathna R, Nagendra HR, Telles S. Progressive increase in critical flicker fusion frequency following yoga training. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol 1997;41:71-4.  Back to cited text no. 15
    
16.Raghuraj P, Telles S. Muscle power, dexterity skill and visual perception in community home girls trained in yoga or sports and in regular school girls. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol 1997;41:409-15.  Back to cited text no. 16
    
17.Panjwani U, Selvamurthy W, Singh SH, Gupta HL, Mukhopadhyay S, Thakur L. Effect of Sahaj yoga meditation on auditory evoked potentials (AEP) and visual contrast sensitivity (VCS) in epileptics. Appl psychophysiol Biofeedback 2000;25:1-2.  Back to cited text no. 17
    
18.Kerr CE, Shaw JR, Wasserman RH, Chen VW, Kanojia A, Bayer T, et al. Tactile acuity in experience Tai Chi practitioners: Evidence for use dependent plasticity as an effect of sensory- attentional training. Exp Brain Res 2008;188:317-22.  Back to cited text no. 18
    
19.Ramdev S. Pranayama Rahasya. Uttarakhand, India: Divya Prakashan; 2009.  Back to cited text no. 19
    
20.Telles S, Raghuraj P, Arankalle D, Naveen KV. Immediate effect of high frequency yoga breathing on attention. Indian J Med Sci 2008;61:20-2.  Back to cited text no. 20
    
21.Telles S, Raghuraj P, Maharana S, Nagendra HR. Immediate effect of three yoga breathing techniques on performance on a letter cancellation task. Percept Mot Skills 2007;104:1289-96.  Back to cited text no. 21
    
22.Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 1971;9:97-113.  Back to cited text no. 22
    
23.Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG. Statistical power analysis using G*Power 3.1: Test for correlation and regression analysis. Behav Res Methods 2009;41:1149-60.  Back to cited text no. 23
    
24.Oxendine JB. Psychology and motor learning. New Jersey , U.S.A.: Prentice Hall; 1984.  Back to cited text no. 24
    
25.Telles S, Raghuraj P, Arankalle D, Naveen KV. Immediate effect of high-frequency yoga breathing on attention. Indian J Med Sci 2008;61:20-2.  Back to cited text no. 25
    
26.Telles S, Maharana K, Balrana B, Acharya Balkrishna. Effects of high frequency yoga breathing called kapalabhati compared with breath awareness on the degree of optical illusion perceived. Percept Mot Skills 2011;112:981-90.  Back to cited text no. 26
    
27.Telles S, Singh N, Balkrishna A. Heart rate variability changes during high frequency yoga breathing and breath awareness. Biopsychosoc Med 2011;5:4.  Back to cited text no. 27
    
28.Joshi M, Telles S. A nonrandomized non-naïve comparative study of the effects of kapalabhati and breath awareness on event-related potentials in trained yoga practitioners. J Altern Complement Med 2009;15:281-5.  Back to cited text no. 28
    
29.Stancak A Jr, Kuna M, Srinivasan, Dostalek C, Vishnudevananda S. Kapalabhati-yogic cleansing exercise. II. EEG topography analysis. Homeost Health Dis 1991;33:182-9.  Back to cited text no. 29
    
30.Brossard-Racine M, Majnemer A, Shevell M, Snider L, Bélanger SA. Handwriting capacity in children newly diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Res Dev Disabil 2011;36:2927-34  Back to cited text no. 30
    
31.Yoshie M, Kudo K, Murakoshi T, Ohsutki T. Music performance anxiety unskilled pianist: Effects of social-evaluative performance situation on subjective, autonomic, and electromyographic reactions. Exp Brain Res 2009;199:117-26.  Back to cited text no. 31
    
32.Lakey CE, Berry DR, Sellers EW. Manipulating attention via mindfulness induction improves P300-based brain-computer interface performance. J Neural Eng 2011;8:025019.  Back to cited text no. 32
    
33.Warnecke E, Quinn S, Ogden K, Towle N, Nelson MR. A randomized controlled trial of the effects of mindfulness practice on medical student stress levels. Med Educ 2011;45:381-8.  Back to cited text no. 33
    
34.Stiles J. Spatial cognitive development In Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience. In: Nelson CA, Luciana M (Editors), Massachusetts, U.S.A.: The MIT Press; 2001.  Back to cited text no. 34
    
35.Telles S, Mohapatra RS, Naveen KV. Heart rate variability spectrum during Vipassana mindfulness meditation. J Indian Psychology 2005;23:1-5.  Back to cited text no. 35
    

Top
Correspondence Address:
Shirley Telles
Patanjali Yogpeeth, Maharishi Dayanand Gram, Near Bahadarabad, Haridwar 249 402, Uttarakhand
India
Login to access the Email id

Source of Support: None, Conflict of Interest: None


DOI: 10.4103/0973-6131.91710

Rights and Permissions



 
 
    Tables

  [Table 1], [Table 2]

This article has been cited by
1 Science of the Mind
Shirley Telles,Nilkamal Singh
Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 2013; 36(1): 93
[Pubmed] | [DOI]



 

Top
 
 
  Search
 
    Similar in PUBMED
    Search Pubmed for
    Search in Google Scholar for
  Related articles
    Email Alert *
    Add to My List *
* Registration required (free)  


    Abstract
   Introduction
    Materials and Me...
   Results
   Discussion
   Conclusion
   Acknowledgement
    References
    Article Tables

 Article Access Statistics
    Viewed3580    
    Printed176    
    Emailed0    
    PDF Downloaded18    
    Comments [Add]    
    Cited by others 1    

Recommend this journal